Autistic peer-to-peer information transfer is highly effective CJ Crompton, D Ropar, CVM Evans-Williams, EG Flynn, ... Autism 24 (7), 1704-1712, 2020 | 455 | 2020 |
Susceptibility to illusions and performance on visuospatial tasks in individuals with autism D Ropar, P Mitchell Journal of child Psychology and Psychiatry 42 (4), 539-549, 2001 | 357 | 2001 |
‘I never realised everybody felt as happy as I do when I am around autistic people’: A thematic analysis of autistic adults’ relationships with autistic and neurotypical … CJ Crompton, S Hallett, D Ropar, E Flynn, S Fletcher-Watson Autism 24 (6), 1438-1448, 2020 | 343 | 2020 |
How easy is it to read the minds of people with autism spectrum disorder? E Sheppard, D Pillai, GTL Wong, D Ropar, P Mitchell Journal of autism and developmental disorders 46, 1247-1254, 2016 | 291 | 2016 |
Are individuals with autism and Asperger's syndrome susceptible to visual illusions? D Ropar, P Mitchell The Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines 40 (8 …, 1999 | 217 | 1999 |
Do gaze cues in complex scenes capture and direct the attention of high functioning adolescents with ASD? Evidence from eye-tracking M Freeth, P Chapman, D Ropar, P Mitchell Journal of autism and developmental disorders 40, 534-547, 2010 | 214 | 2010 |
Shape constancy in autism: The role of prior knowledge and perspective cues D Ropar, P Mitchell Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 43 (5), 647-653, 2002 | 158 | 2002 |
A review of visual perspective taking in autism spectrum disorder A Pearson, D Ropar, AF de C. Hamilton Frontiers in human neuroscience 7, 652, 2013 | 157 | 2013 |
Visuo‐spatial abilities in autism: A review P Mitchell, D Ropar Infant and Child Development: An International Journal of Research and …, 2004 | 156 | 2004 |
Do the eyes have it? Inferring mental states from animated faces in autism E Back, D Ropar, P Mitchell Child development 78 (2), 397-411, 2007 | 153 | 2007 |
Neurotype-matching, but not being autistic, influences self and observer ratings of interpersonal rapport CJ Crompton, M Sharp, H Axbey, S Fletcher-Watson, EG Flynn, D Ropar Frontiers in psychology 11, 586171, 2020 | 146 | 2020 |
Children with autism do not overimitate L Marsh, A Pearson, D Ropar, A Hamilton Current Biology 23 (7), R266-R268, 2013 | 130 | 2013 |
Brief report: Driving hazard perception in autism E Sheppard, D Ropar, G Underwood, E van Loon Journal of autism and developmental disorders 40, 504-508, 2010 | 113 | 2010 |
Measuring the value of social engagement in adults with and without autism I Dubey, D Ropar, AF de C Hamilton Molecular autism 6, 1-9, 2015 | 95 | 2015 |
Susceptibility to the Shepard illusion in participants with autism: Reduced top‐down influences within perception? P Mitchell, L Mottron, I Soulières, D Ropar Autism research 3 (3), 113-119, 2010 | 95 | 2010 |
How perception impacts on drawings. P Mitchell, D Ropar, K Ackroyd, G Rajendran Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 31 (5), 996, 2005 | 93 | 2005 |
Visuo-tactile integration in autism: atypical temporal binding may underlie greater reliance on proprioceptive information K Greenfield, D Ropar, AD Smith, M Carey, R Newport Molecular autism 6, 1-10, 2015 | 84 | 2015 |
Sorting preference in children with autism: The dominance of concrete features D Ropar, D Peebles Journal of autism and developmental disorders 37, 270-280, 2007 | 82 | 2007 |
Spatial transformations of bodies and objects in adults with autism spectrum disorder A Pearson, L Marsh, A Hamilton, D Ropar Journal of autism and developmental disorders 44, 2277-2289, 2014 | 71 | 2014 |
The eye gaze direction of an observed person can bias perception, memory, and attention in adolescents with and without autism spectrum disorder M Freeth, D Ropar, P Chapman, P Mitchell Journal of experimental child psychology 105 (1-2), 20-37, 2010 | 63 | 2010 |